Advanced Search
Current and Breaking News for Professionals, Consumers and Media




Research Author: Staff Editor Last Updated: Sep 7, 2017 - 10:06:33 PM



Fear Factor: Study Shows Brain’s Response to Scary Stimuli

By Staff Editor
Feb 7, 2013 - 12:34:53 PM



Email Newsletter icon, E-mail Newsletter icon, Email List icon, E-mail List icon Sign up for our Ezine
For Email Marketing you can trust


Email this article
 Printer friendly page

(HealthNewsDigest.com) - GAINESVILLE, Fla. - Driving through his hometown, a war veteran with post-traumatic stress disorder may see roadside debris and feel afraid, believing it to be a bomb. He's ignoring his safe, familiar surroundings and only focusing on the debris; yet, when it comes to the visual cortex, a recent study at the University of Florida suggests this is completely normal.

The findings, published in the Journal of Neuroscience, show that even people who don't have anxiety disorders respond visually at the sight of something scary while ignoring signs that indicate safety. This contradicts a common belief that only people with anxiety disorders have difficulty processing comforting visual stimuli, or safety cues, said Andreas Keil, Ph.D., a professor in UF's College of Liberal Arts and Sciences.

"We've established that, in terms of visual responding, it's not a disorder to not respond to a safety cue," Keil said. "We all do that. So now we can study at what stage in the processing stream, with given patients, is the problem occurring."

Co-authors Keil and Vladimir Miskovic, Ph.D., both members of the UF Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention, examined the effect of competing danger and safety cues within the visual cortex. The study results could help distinguish between normal and abnormal processes within the visual cortex and identify what parts of the brain are targets for the treatment of anxiety disorders.

"You'd think the visual cortex would just faithfully code for visual information," said Shmuel Lissek, Ph.D., an assistant professor of psychology at the University of Minnesota. "This kind of work is testing the idea that activations in the visual cortex are actually different if the stimulus has an emotional value than if it doesn't."

For the experiment, 21 undergraduates took turns watching flickering shapes appear on a computer screen while EEG sensors recorded their brainwaves. Through repeated observation, the students learned that one shape was always followed by an unpleasantly loud noise, while another was always followed by silence.

The "loud" shape - the threat cue - appeared and disappeared, and then the "silent" shape - the safety cue - took a turn. The participants learned to fear the "loud" shape, as shown by reactions in their visual cortices.

But when the "loud" and "silent" shapes appeared together on the screen in the second phase of the experiment, the participants' brains reacted the same in response to the "loud" shape. They essentially ignored the "silent" shape, even though it signaled relief from the loud noise. This was determined using a method called frequency tagging, in which the two shapes flicker at different rates, allowing the researchers to isolate and track the responses to both in the visual cortex.

"When threat and safety compete with each other, more attention is still paid to the threat," Keil said. "The visual cortex is very conservative; it's not easily swayed."

Finally, the threat cue shape appeared repeatedly without the bang, until the participant finally trusted that the noise was gone, thus un-learning their fear of the "loud" shape through exposure.

The entire experiment was conducted in about half an hour, although the longer participants are exposed to the loud sound, the more time it takes to eradicate the fear response.

"The longer it takes to learn something, the longer it takes to get rid of it, because it affects a lower and lower level of your brain," said Keil.

In the next stage of their work at the Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention, Miskovic and Keil will study how the brain reacts if participants can take actions to avoid the fear-inducing noise. They will also train participants to alter their fear response in the same way some people can consciously alter their heart rates, which eventually could help people with anxiety disorders control their fear response.

Their work is consistent with the center's basic research aim, said Peter Lang, Ph.D., center director, which is to illuminate the physiology and behavior that characterizes human emotion.

"In this effort, an important focus is the study of brain mechanisms that mediate emotional reactivity," said Lang, a graduate research professor in the department of clinical and health psychology. "The goal is to use the new knowledge to improve the diagnosis and treatment of these mental illnesses likePTSD and other anxiety and mood disorders."

###

For advertising and promotion on www.HealthNewsDigest.com contact Mike McCurdy at: [email protected]  or call 877-634-9180. We are syndicated worldwide and read in 164 countries. We also have over 7,000 journalists as subscribers who may use our content for their own media!

 



Top of Page

HealthNewsDigest.com

Research
Latest Headlines


+ Breaking Research That Could Greatly Improve Transgender Healthcare
+ Pioneering New Gambling Harms Research Centre Launched
+ Checklist for Staying Healthy Overseas
+ Researcher Aims to Protect and Regenerate Corals Through Coral Genomics with $500K NSF Grant and Award-Winning Video
+ Cheers - Five More Years
+ Identify Novel Approach to Preventing Seizures
+ Organ Donation Facts
+ Keeping Web-like NETs from Clogging Blood Vessels Improves Stroke Outcomes
+ Consider Becoming an Organ Donor
+ Tool for Predicting the Future



Contact Us | Job Listings | Help | Site Map | About Us
Advertising Information | HND Press Release | Submit Information | Disclaimer

Site hosted by Sanchez Productions